eISSN: 2720-5371
ISSN: 1230-2813
Advances in Psychiatry and Neurology/Postępy Psychiatrii i Neurologii
Bieżący numer Archiwum Artykuły zaakceptowane O czasopiśmie Rada naukowa Bazy indeksacyjne Prenumerata Kontakt Zasady publikacji prac Opłaty publikacyjne Standardy etyczne i procedury
Panel Redakcyjny
Zgłaszanie i recenzowanie prac online
SCImago Journal & Country Rank
3/2021
vol. 30
 
Poleć ten artykuł:
Udostępnij:
Artykuł przeglądowy

Aleksytymia u pacjentów po udarze mózgu – przegląd literatury

Piotr Leszczyński
1
,
Tadeusz Pietras
1
,
Łukasz Mokros
1

  1. Department of Clinical Pharmacology, Medical University of Lodz, Poland
Adv Psychiatry Neurol 2021; 30 (3): 190-196
Data publikacji online: 2021/11/26
Plik artykułu:
- 00157.pdf  [0.13 MB]
Pobierz cytowanie
 
Metryki PlumX:
 

INTRODUCTION

In the human population there are persons who are little interested in their own emotional life, and, moreover, are not able to recognize and delineate their states to others. For such an inability the term alexithymia was proposed by Sifneos in 1973 [1], and has been further investigated by many researchers. It has been proved that alexithymia is a stable personality trait [2] and is characterized by four different facets: difficulty identifying feelings (DIF), difficulty describing feelings (DDF), restricted imagination including the paucity of fantasies, and externally oriented cognitive style (EOT) [3]. Those deficits are often misinterpreted as somatic complaints [1]. To evaluate the level of alexithymia self-report scales are mainly used, among which TAS-20 (Toronto Alexithymia Scale) is the most widely applied in clinical work. It has been translated to more than 20 languages, with a score of ≥ 61, which reflects the presence of alexithymia. Although TAS-20 is validated and reliable [2, 4], the usage of such instruments imposes limitations on the measurement, with subjectivity being an important flaw.

The prevalence of alexithymia in the general population, according to different studies, varies from 5.2% to 17% [5-8], with most researchers indicating that men not only score higher in the measures of the trait, but that it is also more common among them than it is among women [5-11]. It is debatable if such sex differences, especially regarding EOT facet, are due to cultural impacts and gender role socialisation [12, 13] or some other factors. The linkage between ageing and alexithymia shows discrepancies, with some studies showing no associations [5] while others suggest that alexithymia increases with age [6, 7, 11], and yet others that it decreases [4, 14]. The incremental rise in alexithymia with age has been explained by the poor physical health of older people and differences in upbringing and cultural environment among them and the young population, which leads to disparate coping styles regarding their affects, with a strong correlation between aging and alexithymia shown in the study by Mattila et al. [11]. On the other hand, very low correlations were observed regarding no association of linkage of alexithymia and age as well as alexithymia decreasing with age [5, 14].

Alexithymia is associated with language deficits and, as observed in many psychiatric and somatic conditions, its presence should be taken into account while a choosing proper form of treatment and therapy, with psychodynamic therapy being an inappropriate form of treatment for such patients [15]. It also plays an important role in prognosis and therapeutic decisions for patients with neurological diseases, and its prevalence among stroke survivors is a common feature [16].

THE “ORGANIC ALEXITHYMIA” CONCEPT AND ITS OUTCOMES

The concept of splitting alexithymia on the basis of its derivation appeared early in the literature, and the terms “primary” and “secondary” emerged consequently [17], later to be complemented with the term “normative”, which encompasses the differences in alexithymia between men and women posited to be caused by traditional masculine role socialization [12].

Primary alexithymia refers to a stable personality trait that is developed in early childhood and early adult years [18], with some causes of it being psychic trauma [19], negative attachment experiences [20] and genetic factors [9], such as genetic polymorphism of the 5-HT transporter-linked promoter region [21].

Secondary alexithymia, on the other hand, is a result of events that occur in the life course, both of psychological significance (e.g. adult trauma) or chronic medical illness, with its foundation lying in psychological and somatic (organic) causes [18]. Therefore, secondary alexithymia is a consequence of a disease, and while it might be serving as a defence mechanism to help in coping with stress and highly emotional events [22], it also can be a result of a brain injury (both direct and indirect insults to the brain) that leads to damage of the regions of the nervous tissue responsible for emotional processing in the right hemisphere [23, 24]. Therefore, the term “organic alexithymia” has been introduced for such nervous tissue aftermath insults, and was described in a detailed way in 2002 by Becerra et al. [24], and although it had been used before in 1990 by Savard and Andermann [25], its widespread use among researchers began with Becerra’s and his colleagues’ descriptive work.

Regarding brain structures and the damage to them responsible for the occurrence of secondary alexithymia, a few of them seem to play a decisive role. In their work on neurobiological underpinnings of alexithymia, Meza-Concha et al. [26] reviewed multiple papers concerning alexithymia and its neurobiological coordinates, mentioning damage to such structures as the amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), anterior insula (AI), pre-frontal cortex (PFC), cerebellum, and processes of abused interhemispheric connections being responsible for the occurrence of the trait. Patrikelis et al. [27], who also reviewed multiple papers regarding alexithymia’s neurobiological coordinates, conclude that insulting the anterior cortical midline structures like the aforementioned ACC, AI and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) can possibly play key roles in secondary alexithymia, as these structures take part in regulating various processes of our emotional life. Through regulating the interoceptive processing (receiving and transforming bodily states into feelings), AI regulates the conscious self-awareness of emotions [28]; ACC regulates the process of interoceptive awareness and mediates the appearance of emotional arousal, and along with mPFC regulates the limbic structures involved in generating emotions [27]. Papers reviewed in abovementioned studies on finding the damage to exact brain regions responsible for alexithymic traits were mostly based on careful examination of neuroimaging, including MRI, fMRI, PET and others. Worth mentioning is the fact that activation of the AI, ACC and mPFC is linked to, among other things, altruistic behaviour, empathic concern and perspective taking [29].

Organic alexithymia is common in many neurological conditions (e.g. various forms of dementia, stroke, epilepsy, traumatic brain injury, Parkinson’s disease) due to alterations to the brain regions responsible for emotional awareness, mainly in the right hemisphere [16]. It coexists with cognitive impairment and is considered by some to be more resistant to treatment than other forms of alexithymia [30]. Worth mentioning is the classification of alexithymia based on clinical history, as proposed by Messina and colleagues [18], who stated that responses to treatment and outcomes in patients with organic alex-ithymia are better than in those suffering from the primary nature of the trait, but it must be underlined that authors emphasise the fact that this was based on the research studies and personal experience of one of them. Moreover, as underlined in their research, the involvement of the brain structures responsible for alexithymia (both primary and organic) cannot be used to differentiate the origin of the trait, and instead clinical symptoms and outcomes should be applied to distinguish one from another, with further research focusing on this subject required. Taking the above into consideration, clinical decisions regarding patients suffering from organic diseases should be examined with caution, depending on whether alexithymia derived from primary cause and has existed beforehand in such a patient, or is an acquired trait due to a brain illness and/or insult.

In their paper, Ricciardi et al. [16] suggested that alexithymia among patients with neurological diseases might be a manifestation of right hemispheric denial syndrome, as there is no control over the overall denial style in TAS-20. They also stressed that alexithymia is hard to distinguish from other subjective facets which concern feelings and that it might resemble aprosodic deficit, which is an inability to recognise and express affective or emotional information carried by the voice, which features, among other things, pitch, tone, rate and loudness, with emotional prosody being almost fully functional [31-33]. Alexithymia, on the other hand, is a global cognitive-affective deficit that concerns emotional awareness, diminished interest of one’s own emotional life, and a paucity of fantasies.

Riccardi et al. suggest that such knowledge should be taken into consideration when assessing the trait among the aprosodic population. Their conclusion regarding the aforementioned issues is that TAS-20 should be contextually evaluated for patients suffering from various forms of neuropsychological deficits and that further researchers should consider switching to a more objective evaluation scale (i.e. Alexithymia Provoked Response Questionnaire).

ALEXITHYMIA AMONG STROKE SURVIVORS

A small number of studies show a linkage between alexithymia and psychiatric comorbidity following stroke. Among them is one from Wang et al. [34], who examined ninety patients (78 of them completing the study) with stroke, looking at associations of post-stroke alexithymia with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and psychiatric comorbidity. They discovered that at approximately 1 month after the stroke, alexithymia was positively correlated with severity of PTSD and psychiatric comorbidity, with DIF having the strongest association. They also found (controlling for variables of PTSD, physical disability, psychiatric comorbidity and the time elapsed since the occur-rence of the stroke) that alexithymia was not associated with the aforementioned after three months following stroke, therefore, its influence over time disappeared. In another study of psychiatric comorbidity and alexithymia following stroke Hung et al. [35] investigated the role of alexithymia in post-stroke depression (PSD). 285 patients were enrolled in the study, with 93.3% completing it. The authors proved that higher levels of the alexithymia at the baseline of occurrence of the stroke, especially the difficulty in identifying emotions, were predictive factors for PSD in the next six months.

Bossu et al. investigated the association between disease outcome, alexithymia, depression and serum levels of pro-inflammatory cytokine interleukin-18 (IL-18) among stroke survivors. The findings of their study were that the IL-18 level increases specifically in patients with post-stroke alexithymia, and does not increase in healthy subjects and non-alexithymic patients. Also, a positive correlation was found between levels of cytokine and the trait, especially among patients with right brain damage (RBD). Moreover, alexithymic patients had more severe stroke in the acute phase than those who were non-alexithymic, with IL-18 not being correlated with stroke severity. The authors point to the clinical usage of their findings, proposing IL-18 as a factor of emotional awareness disturbances among stroke survivors [36]. Their study was later extended to the one conducted by Sacchinelli et al., which showed that among healthy subjects there is a significant correlation between IL-18 levels and the alexithymic facet of having difficulties identifying feelings [37].

In 2001 Spalletta and colleagues conducted a study examining the effects of laterality and gender regarding alexithymic features in forty-eight post-stroke patients. Patients with right brain damage (RBD) were more prone to develop alexithymia than those with left brain damage (LBD). Analysis of the data showed that only describing and identifying feelings, not the EOT facet, could distinguish patients with RBD and LBD, which leads to the conclusion that some – but not all – aspects of ale xithymia are connected to the right hemisphere. The study also confirmed a previously reported discovery that alexithymia and depression are distinctive dimensionally, although alexithymia is a risk factor for post-stroke depression. Regarding gender differences, men with RBD scored higher on TAS-20 than men with LBD, but this observation did not apply to women, among whom both groups had high level of alexithymia [23].

In 2006 the same researchers examined fifty patients with a first-ever stroke diagnosis and a diagnosis of major depressive disorder towards alexithymia. Eighteen of them, with lesions more frequent in right hemisphere, proved to have alexithymia, as validated with TAS-20. Subjects were administered antidepressant therapy (sertraline or fluoxetine), and reassessed after eight weeks of treatment. The data collected showed that among non- alexithymia patients no significant changes were evident, but that among patients with alexithymia the decline in severity of the trait, unconnected to response to depression, was observed both with the identifying and describing of feelings, but not with the EOT facet. Surprisingly, the cognitive impairment measured by MMSE (Mini Mental State Examination) improved only in patients without alexithymia. The authors presume this was because of the low EOT facet interfering with cognitive recovery [38]. Similar conclusions regarding alexithymia and its treatment come from the study by Cravello et al., who found that both fluoxetine and venlafaxine lower alexithymia among patients with post-stroke depression, the latter being more effective than the former, which leads to the further conclusion that not only serotonergic, but also adrenergic transmission prove to have an impact on alexithymia among post-stroke patients [39].

Limitations of Spalletta’s et al. studies were small groups of participants and exclusion of individuals with aphasia, which might back up the RBD as an important factor of organic alexithymia, since the patients with acquired language problems due to LBD embracing language areas would have been rejected, so further evaluation of the results should be conducted.

Using PET and self-report scales (including TAS-20), Paradiso et al. examined the effects of middle cerebral artery (MCA) stroke on emotional activity in neural areas distant from the insult. After the stroke, the anterior cingu-late cortex (ACC) showed decreased neural activity, which resulted in reduced emotional awareness, thus inducing alexithymia among subjects. Moreover, the EOT facet did not decline in stroke subjects, so the findings were consistent with those by Spalletta et al. [38]. The authors stress that presentation of depression might be altered among alexithymic patients, therefore proper instructions should be given to such patients and their families concerning observing symptoms for the assessment of the hedonic tone of situations [40]. The sample of this study was relatively small (6 stroke subjects and 12 healthy volunteers), so further research is needed to confirm the findings.

In their recent study (2020), Hobson et al. examined the question of whether organic alexithymia was somehow determined by acquired language problems among chronic stroke patients. A sample of 118 patients was included, both with and without post-stroke language difficulties, and to overcome the problem of language-impaired patients self-reported data was supervised by a trained speech and language therapist. The research concluded that while patients with self-reported communication difficulties were prone to alexithymia, those with language-impairment and non-impairment that were tested with behavioural measures showed no difference in alexithymia levels. The authors emphasize the point that such results might be the cause of less sensitivity in behavioural measures than self-report tools. They also point out that future research is needed concerning short-term language impairments as being the cause of alexithymia, as they conducted their research at 6 months or later post-stroke. Contradictory to previously mentioned studies, they found that both RBD and LBD were the same risk factors for acquiring alexithymia [41]. Similarly, Paradiso et al. found, in their study of subjects with left basal ganglia stroke, that compared to healthy subjects patients with LBD scored higher on TAS-20. To explain this phenomenon they proposed that activity changes in the right hemisphere following LBD might help explain such observations [42]. There have also been studies which suggested that alexithymia appears due to the poor transfer of emotional information from right hemisphere to language regions in the left hemisphere [43-45].

DISCUSSION

Alexithymia is a common clinical problem among stroke survivors [23, 35, 36, 38, 39, 41]. Its occurrence among this population of patients is associated with elevated levels of interleukin-18 [36] and damage to brain structures which lie in the right hemisphere, mainly the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), anterior insula (AI) and prefrontal cortex (PFC) [23, 26, 27], though insults to left hemisphere can also play an important, yet still not fully understood role [41, 42]. Alexithymia has a positive correlation with the severity of post-stroke PTSD [34] and is a predictive factor for depression in stroke survivors [35]. Venlafaxine, fluoxetine and sertraline should be considered for lowering the degree of alexithymia among post-stroke depression population, but little evidence is available so far [38, 39].

The papers mentioned have some limitations, which should be undoubtedly taken into consideration in further research. The exclusion of patients who have suffered from communication issues, especially those with LBD, makes studies less precise and does not show how many patients really suffer from post-stroke alexithymia and how LBD and language difficulties can affect the prevalence of the trait in this population; this is the case with the papers by Wang et al., Spalletta et al., Hung et al. [23, 34, 35, 38]. Hobson et al. [41] tried to overcome that problem through the oversight of a trained speech and language therapist in the assessment of the trait, yet their sample consisted of patients who had a stroke 6-months prior to the screening, thus some of their symptoms may have partially resolved and other vascular changes might develop [42].

Small sample size, as well as limitations on the type of stroke included, are factors that should make the studies by Hung et al., Spalletta et al., Bossu et al., Cravello et al. and Paradiso et al. [23, 35, 36, 38-40] considered as preliminary, with stronger evidence embracing the broader range of locations of the lesions and choice of treatment is needed to confirm their findings. Another important thing that future researchers should take into consideration is the involvement of interhemispheric transfer in the role of alexithymia, bearing in mind that patients with communication difficulties should also be enrolled in such studies. Only a few studies have shown that LBD can also play an important role in the occurrence of the trait [41, 42], which can lead to the conclusion that abnormal information transfer between hemispheres is underestimated in research following organic alexithymia among stroke survivors, even though it seems to play an important role in the explanation of its origins.

CONClUSIONS

Alexithymia is a prevalent issue among stroke-survivors. The ubiquitous assessment of the trait in this population ought to be considered, as alexithymia plays an important role in psychiatric comorbidity and severity of the stroke itself, and might indirectly interfere with the rehabilitation process [23]. Pharmacological treatment might help lower the alexithymia levels in post-stroke subjects, but there is little data on which drugs are preferable. The possible choice of psychotherapy should be adopted with caution, as psychodynamic therapy is contraindicated against patients with alexithymia. Further research is required to examine alexithymia’s outcomes among patients with left brain stroke damage, its impact on other post-stroke psychiatric comorbidities, and to closely determine the best options for rehabilitation, pharmacological treatment, and psychological therapy.

Conflict of interest/Konflikt interesu

Absent./Nie występuje.

Financial support/Finansowanie

Absent./Nie występuje.

References

1 

Sifneos PE. The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom 1973; 22: 255-262.

2 

Mikolajczak M, Luminet O. Is alexithymia affected by situational stress or is it a stable trait related to emotion regulation? Pers Individ Differ 2006; 40: 1399-1408.

3 

Taylor GJ, Michael Bagby R, Parker JDA. The alexithymia construct: a potential paradigm for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatics 1991; 32: 153-164.

4 

Parker JDA, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. The 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. J Psychosom Res 2003; 55: 269-275.

5 

Franz M, Popp K, Schaefer R, Sitte W, Schneider C, Hardt J, et al. Alexithymia in the German general population. Soc Psychiat Epidemiol 2008; 43: 54-62.

6 

Salminen JK, Saarijärvi S, Äärelä E, Toikka T, Kauhanen J. Prevalence of alexithymia and its association with sociodemographic variables in the general population of Finland. J Psychosom Res 1999; 46: 75-82.

7 

Honkalampi K, Hintikka J, Tanskanen A, Lehtonen J, Viinamäki H. Depression is strongly associated with alexithymia in the general population. J Psychosom Res 2000; 48: 99-104.

8 

Kokkonen P, Karvonen JT, Veijola J, Läksy K, Jokelainen J, Järvelin MR, et al. Prevalence and sociodemographic correlates of alexithymia in a population sample of young adults. Compr Psychiatry 2001; 42: 471-476.

9 

Jørgensen MM, Zachariae R, Skytthe A, Kyvik K. Genetic and environmental factors in alexithymia: a population-based study of 8,785 Danish twin pairs. Psychother Psychosom 2007; 76: 369-375.

10 

Karukivi M, Jula A, Hutri-Kähönen N, Juonala M, Raitakari O. Is alexithymia associated with metabolic syndrome? A study in a healthy adult population. Psychiatry Res 2016; 236: 58-63.

11 

Mattila AK, Salminen JK, Nummi T, Joukamaa M. Age is strongly associated with alexithymia in the general population. J Psychosom Res 2006; 61: 629-635.

12 

Levant RF, Hall RJ, Williams CM, Hasan NT. Gender differences in alexithymia. Psychology of Men & Masculinity 2009; 10: 190-203.

13 

Besharat MA. Reliability and factorial validity of a Farsi version of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale with a sample of Iranian Students. Psychol Rep 2007; 101: 209-220.

14 

Moriguchi Y, Maeda M, Igarashi T, Ishikawa T, Shoji M, Kubo C, Komaki G. Age and gender effect on alexithymia in large, Japanese community and clinical samples: a cross-validation study of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20). Biopsychosoc Med 2007; 1: 7.

15 

Pinna F, Manchia M, Paribello P, Carpiniello B. The impact of alexithymia on treatment response in psychiatric disorders: a systematic review. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11: 311.

16 

Ricciardi L, Demartini B, Fotopoulou A, Edwards MJ. Alexithymia in neurological disease: a review. J Neuro-psych Clin Neurosci 2015; 27: 179-187.

17 

Freyberger H. Supportive psychotherapeutic techniques in primary and secondary alexithymia. Psychother Psychosom 1977; 28: 337-342.

18 

Messina A, Beadle JN, Paradiso S. Towards a classification of alexithymia: primary, secondary and organic. J Psychopathol 2014; 20: 38-49.

19 

Zlotnick C, Mattia JI, Zimmerman M. The relationship between posttraumatic stress disorder, childhood trauma and alexithymia in an outpatient sample. J Traum Stress 2001; 14: 177-188.

20 

Besharat MA, Khajavi Z. The relationship between attachment styles and alexithymia: mediating role of defense mechanisms. Asian J Psychiatr 2013; 6: 571-576.

21 

Kano M, Mizuno T, Kawano Y, Aoki M, Kanazawa M, Fukudo S. Serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism and alexithymia. Neuropsychobiology 2012; 65: 76-82.

22 

Wise TN, Mann LS, Mitchell JD, Hryvniak M, Hill B. Secondary alexithymia: an empirical validation. Compr Psychiatry 1990; 31: 284-288.

23 

Spalletta G, Pasini A, Costa A, De Angelis D, Ramundo N, Paolucci S, et al. Alexithymic features in stroke: effects of laterality and gender. Psychosom Med 2001; 63: 944-950.

24 

Becerra R, Amos A, Jongenelis S. Organic alexithymia: a study of acquired emotional blindness. Brain Inj 2002; 16: 633-645.

25 

Savard G, Andermann F. Convulsive pseudoseizures: a review of current concepts. Behav Neurol 1990; 3: 133-141.

26 

Meza-Concha N, Arancibia M, Salas F, Behar R, Salas G, Silva H, et al. Towards a neurobiological understanding of alexithymia. Medwave 2017; 17: e6960-e6960.

27 

Patrikelis P, Lucci G, Alexoudi A, Korfias S, Messinis L, Nasios G, et al. Addressing evidence linking secondary alexithymia to aberrant humor processing. Behav Neurol 2019; 2019: 1-13.

28 

Hogeveen J, Bird G, Chau A, Krueger F, Grafman J. Acquired alexithymia following damage to the anterior insula. Neuropsychologia 2016; 82: 142-148.

29 

Chau A, Zhong W, Gordon B, Krueger F, Grafman J. Anterior insula lesions and alexithymia reduce the endorsements of everyday altruistic attitudes. Neuropsychologia 2018; 117: 428-439.

30 

Huang MF, Yeh YC, Tsang HY, Chen CS. Alexithymia associated with bilateral globus pallidus lesions after carbon monoxide poisoning. Kaohsiung J Med Sci 2010; 26: 333-336.

31 

Jones M, Jorge RE. Depression and Other Neuropsychiatric Issues Following Stroke. In: Stroke Rehabilitation [Internet]. Elsevier; 2019, pp. 155-168. Available at: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/B9780323553810000111 (Accessed: 23.02.2021).

32 

Leon SA, Rodriguez AD. Aprosodia and its treatment. Perspect Neurophysiol Neurogenic Speech Lang Disord 2008; 18: 66-72.

33 

Darby DG. Sensory aprosodia: a clinical clue to lesions of the inferior division of the right middle cerebral artery? Neurology 1993; 43 (3 Part 1): 567-567.

34 

Wang X, Chung MC, Hyland ME, Bahkeit M. Posttraumatic stress disorder and psychiatric co-morbidity following stroke: the role of alexithymia. Psychiatry Res 2011; 188: 51-57.

35 

Hung TH, Chou SY, Su JA. The role of alexithymia in the incidence of poststroke depression. J Nerv Ment Dis 2015; 203: 966-970.

36 

Bossu P, Salani F, Cacciari C, Picchetto L, Cao M, Bizzoni F, et al. Disease outcome, alexithymia and depression are differently associated with serum IL-18 levels in acute stroke. Curr Neurovasc Res 2009; 6: 163-170.

37 

Sacchinelli E, Piras F, Orfei MD, Banaj N, Salani F, Ciaramella A, et al. IL-18 serum levels and variants of the serotonin transporter gene are related to awareness of emotions in healthy subjects: a preliminary study. Neuroimmunomodulation 2018; 25: 129-137.

38 

Spalletta G, Ripa A, Bria P, Caltagirone C, Robinson RG. Response of emotional unawareness after stroke to antidepressant treatment. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006; 14: 220-227.

39 

Cravello L, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G. The SNRI venlafaxine improves emotional unawareness in patients with post-stroke depression. Hum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp 2009; 24: 331-336.

40 

Paradiso S, Anderson BM, Boles Ponto LL, Tranel D, Robinson RG. Altered neural activity and emotions following right middle cerebral artery stroke. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis 2011; 20: 94-104.

41 

Hobson H, Chiu EG, Ravenscroft C, Partridge K, Bird G, Demeyere N. The association between communication impairments and acquired alexithymia in chronic stroke patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2020; 42: 495-504.

42 

Paradiso S, Ostedgaard K, Vaidya J, Ponto LB, Robinson R. Emotional blunting following left basal ganglia stroke: The role of depression and fronto-limbic functional alterations. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2013; 211: 148-159.

43 

Jessimer M, Markham R. Alexithymia: a right hemisphere dysfunction specific to recognition of certain facial expressions? Brain Cogn 1997; 34: 246-258.

44 

Miller L. Is alexithymia a disconnection syndrome? A neuropsychological perspective. Int J Psychiatry Med 1987; 16: 199-209.

45 

Tabibnia G, Zaidel E. Alexithymia, interhemispheric transfer, and right hemispheric specialization: a critical review. Psychother Psychosom 2005; 74: 81-92.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0). License allowing third parties to download and share its works but not commercially purposes or to create derivative works.
 
© 2024 Termedia Sp. z o.o.
Developed by Bentus.