eISSN: 1509-572x
ISSN: 1641-4640
Folia Neuropathologica
Current issue Archive Manuscripts accepted About the journal Special Issues Editorial board Reviewers Abstracting and indexing Subscription Contact Instructions for authors Ethical standards and procedures
Editorial System
Submit your Manuscript
SCImago Journal & Country Rank
1/2022
vol. 60
 
Share:
Share:
Original paper

Correlation between focal lesion sites and language deficits in the acute phase of post-stroke aphasia

Qing-qing Liu
1
,
Wei-bo Li
2
,
Yue-wen Song
3
,
Zhen-biao Zhao
3
,
Yan-ning Yan
3
,
Yu-hui Yang
3
,
Pei-yuan Lv
4
,
Yu Yin
1, 3

1.
Graduate School of Hebei Medical University, Shijiazhuang, China
2.
Department of Gastrointestinal Surgery, the Second Hospital of Hebei Medical University, Shijiazhuang, China
3.
Department of Rehabilitation, Hebei General Hospital, Shijiazhuang, China
4.
Department of Neurology, Hebei General Hospital, Shijiazhuang, China
Folia Neuropathol 2022; 60 (1): 60-68
Online publish date: 2022/03/29
Article files
- Correlation.pdf  [0.11 MB]
Get citation
 
PlumX metrics:
 

Introduction

Aphasia is a language disorder mainly caused by stroke. Approximately 21% to 38% of stroke patients suffer from aphasia, which affects not only their daily communication, but also their social activities and quality of life [7]. The brain is a complex network structure that uses multiple regions to perform different tasks, especially language tasks. Studies have shown that performing a language task requires the participation of multiple language-related brain regions, which means that different language regions may have different roles [24,29,30,36]. Therefore, damage to the focal language regions of the brain may cause specific language deficits. Researchers have also emphasised that understanding the role of the specific brain regions related to language after aphasia can help in understanding the pathophysiological mechanism of aphasia and serve as the basis for neuroregulatory strategies in aphasia [35].
Traditionally, Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area have been regarded as the classical language areas that play an important role in fluency and comprehension, respectively. However, existing studies have shown that other parts of these areas, such as the insula, also contribute to language deficits [6,14]. This indicates that understanding the role of broader language areas is crucial.
Recent studies have shown that focal lesion sites are related to specific language deficits [1,2,17]. For example, one study has shown that damage to the inferior triangularis and inferior operculum of the frontal cortex, supramarginal cortex and insula results in poor fluency and that lesions involving the parietal cortex, angular cortex, temporal middle cortex, sagittal stratum and temporal superior cortex are associated with poor comprehension [35]. How- ever, these studies explored the relationship between lesion sites and language deficits in the subacute or chronic phases, while this relationship in the acute phase is still unknown. Therefore, the purpose of our study is to investigate the relationship between focal lesion sites and specific language deficits in the acute phase based on imaging data obtained within 1-2 days after stroke.

Material and methods

Participants

We retrospectively collected the data of individuals admitted to the Hebei General Hospital between June 2018 and June 2021. The inclusion criteria were as follows: 1) first-ever stroke, 2) aphasia, 3) lesions confined to the left hemisphere, 4) acute phase ≤ 1 month, 5) native Chinese speaker, 6) right-handed, 7) age ≥ 18 years and 8) imaging data obtained by either magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) or computed tomography (CT) within 1-2 days after stroke. Patients were excluded if they had a prior history of psychological or neurological disease. We had no restrictions on the severity of the aphasia or type of aphasia. We followed the principles of the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology initiative (Supplementary Appendix A) [37].

Measures

Demographic information, including age, sex and education level, was extracted. Clinical features, including the time since onset, type of stroke (infarction or haemorrhage), type of aphasia (Broca’s aphasia, Wernicke’s aphasia, global aphasia, etc.), risk factors, stroke severity, language function, lesion site, dysphagia, dysarthria and cognitive impairment, were also extracted. The risk factors mainly involved high blood pressure, diabetes, hyperlipidaemia, coronary heart disease, atrial fibrillation, smoking and drinking.
Stroke severity was assessed by the Chinese version of the National Institute of Health Stroke Scale within 1-2 days after stroke. The scale consists of 15 items, including the level of consciousness, eye movements, integrity of visual fields, facial movements, arm and leg muscle strength, sensation, coordination, language, speech and neglect [21]. The score for each item ranges from 0 to 2, 0 to 3, or 0 to 4, and the total score ranges from 0 to 42. The higher the score, the more severe the stroke.
Language function was assessed by the Chinese version of the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB) scale when the patients came to the rehabilitation department within one month after stroke [34]. We mainly included fluency, auditory comprehension, repetition, naming ability and the aphasia quotient (AQ) in our study. The AQ was defined as the severity of the aphasia, ranging from 0 to 100. It was obtained by calculation. The formula was as follows: (spontaneous speech + auditory comprehension/20 + repetition/10 + naming/10) multiplied by 2.
Three professional speech and language pathologists blind to the results of the patients’ language function independently assessed the lesion sites based on their experience and the patients’ imaging data. Recognition of the lesion sites was mainly based on the main sulcus of the brain. During the above process, differing opinions were resolved through negotiation. The imaging data were derived from high-resolution MRI or CT scans. The main areas of interest related to language that we chose were the superior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, postcentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus and insula.
Dysphagia was identified by the water swallow test [12]. Patients were given 3 oz of water and asked to drink from a cup without interruption. Coughing during the test or for one minute after completion or the presence of a post-swallow wet hoarse voice quality was scored as abnormal.
Dysarthria was assessed by the Chinese version of the Frenchay Dysarthria Assessment 2 [19]. This is a standardised tool for diagnosing dysarthria and contains seven sections: reflexes, respiration, laryngeal, lips, palate, tongue and intelligibility. Each section includes items that are assessed at rest, in motion and during speech. The result of each item contains five grades ranging from A to E, with A being considered normal and E considered the worst.
Cognitive impairment was assessed by the Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination scale, which includes orientation, memory, attention and computation, recall and language ability [3]. A score of 25-30 is considered normal, and a score < 25 is considered to indicate cognitive deficits.

Statistical analysis

A descriptive analysis was used to analyse the baseline indicators of the patients, including their age, sex, education level, time since onset, type of stroke, type of aphasia, risk factors, stroke severity, dysphagia, dysarthria, cognitive impairment and language function. Since the age, time since onset, stroke severity and language function were continuous data, we used the mean (standard deviation) or median (interquartile range) to express these results.
The quantitative analysis methods used included the Wilcoxon rank sum test, the Kruskal-Wallis H test, Spearman’s rank correlation test and multivariate linear regression analysis. The Wilcoxon rank sum test was used to analyse the relationship between lesion sites and fluency, auditory comprehension, repetition and naming deficits because each language score comprised continuous data and did not conform to the normal distribution. In order to further explore the influence of other variables on aphasia, the Wilcoxon rank sum test was used to explore the relationship between sex, type of stroke, risk factors, dysphagia, dysarthria and cognitive impairment and the AQ. The Kruskal–Wallis H test was used to explore the relationship between education level and the AQ. Spearman’s rank correlation test was used to explore the relationship between age, stroke severity and total number of language-related lesion sites and the AQ. Multivariate linear regression analysis was used for risk factors that significantly affected AQ. The SPSS 21.0 software was used for the statistical analysis. A P-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

A total of 383 patients with aphasia were admitted to the rehabilitation department from June 2018 to June 2021. According to the inclusion and exclusion criteria, 52 patients were included in total. The reasons for exclusion in the screening process were as follows: 19 patients were non-stroke, 90 patients were non-first-stroke, 3 patients were not right-handed, 204 patients had no initial imaging data, 5 patients had lesions involving the right cerebral hemisphere, and 10 patients were not in the acute phase (Fig. 1).
Regarding the 52 patients, the mean age was 57.40 (13.10). The gender distribution was 35 males, accounting for 67%, and 17 females, accounting for 33%. In terms of the education level, 8 patients had a bachelor’s degree, 14 patients had a high school degree, 16 patients had a junior high school degree, 11 patients had a primary school degree, and 3 patients were illiterate. The median time since onset was 15.50 (9.00; 21.00) days. Regarding the type of stroke, 36 patients had cerebral infarction, accounting for 69%, and 16 patients had cerebral haemorrhage, accounting for 31%. For the type of aphasia, 12 patients had Broca’s aphasia, 4 patients had Wernicke’s aphasia, 3 patients had conduction aphasia, 1 patient had transcortical motor aphasia, 3 patients had transcortical sensory aphasia, 3 patients had mixed transcortical aphasia, 8 patients had anomia aphasia, 13 patients had global aphasia, and 5 patients had unclassified aphasia. Among the risk factors we included, hypertension was the most common risk factor, with 36 of 52 patients suffering from it. The stroke severity was assessed in only 32 of the 52 patients, with a median score of 4.50 (3.25; 10.75). Finally, 10 patients had dysphagia (accounting for 19%), 5 patients had dysarthria (accounting for 10%), and 6 patients had cognitive impairment (accounting for 12%) (Table I).
The results of the Wilcoxon rank sum test showed that lesions involving the superior temporal gyrus (p = 0.025), middle frontal gyrus (p = 0.014), inferior frontal gyrus (p = 0.002), precentral gyrus (p = 0.004), postcentral gyrus (p = 0.037), supramarginal gyrus (p = 0.043), angular gyrus (p = 0.044) and insula (p = 0.025) were significantly associated with low fluency. Lesions involving the superior temporal gyrus (p = 0.010), middle temporal gyrus (p = 0.035), inferior temporal gyrus (p = 0.032), middle frontal gyrus (p = 0.016), inferior frontal gyrus (p = 0.006), supramarginal gyrus (p = 0.002) and angular gyrus (p = 0.009) significantly resulted in auditory comprehension impairment. Lesions involving the superior temporal gyrus (p = 0.009), middle temporal gyrus (p = 0.037), middle frontal gyrus (p = 0.015), inferior frontal gyrus (p = 0.001), precentral gyrus (p = 0.012), postcentral gyrus (p = 0.006), supramarginal gyrus (p = 0.023), angular gyrus (p = 0.003) and insula (p = 0.018) significantly resulted in repetition damage. Lesions involving the superior temporal gyrus (p = 0.034), middle temporal gyrus (p = 0.034), middle frontal gyrus (p = 0.007), inferior frontal gyrus (p = 0.001), precentral gyrus (p = 0.007), postcentral gyrus (p = 0.017), supramarginal gyrus (p = 0.022), angular gyrus (p = 0.014) and insula (p = 0.004) significantly caused a low naming ability (Table II).
The patients’ age, sex, education level, type of stroke, risk factors included, stroke severity and cognitive impairment were not significantly related to the AQ, while dysphagia (p < 0.001), dysarthria (p = 0.009) and the total number of language-related lesion sites (p < 0.001) were significantly related to a low AQ (Table III). Multivariate linear regression analysis further showed that dysphagia and the total number of laguage-related lesion sites were significantly related with a low AQ, while dysarthria was not related with AQ.

Discussion

Our study found that the focal lesion locations were related to specific language deficits in the patients with acute aphasia after stroke (≤ 1 month). Damage to the superior temporal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, postcentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus and insula was associated with poor fluency. Lesions including the superior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, supramarginal gyrus and angular gyrus were associated with poor auditory comprehension. Lesions involving the superior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, postcentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus and insula were associated with poor repetition and poor naming ability. These findings suggested that the execution of each language task may require the participation of multiple language regions and that each language region may also participate in multiple language tasks. That is, language is a complex system, and each language task is completed via the cooperation of multiple language functions. Therefore, the injury of any function will lead to certain language deficits, which helps us further understand the neural mechanism of aphasia. Moreover, our study showed that the severity of aphasia in the acute phase was not associated with age, sex, education level, type of stroke, risk factors included, stroke severity or whether there was cognitive impairment. However, if a patient had dysphagia, dysarthria or a higher number of language-related lesion sites, the degree of aphasia in the acute phase was more serious.
Fluency involves a series of cognitive functions, including semantic memory, grammatical knowledge, working memory, articulatory planning and executive function. In one study [28], in order to explore the influencing factors of fluency, the deficits were simplified and divided into articulatory deficits and grammatical deficits. Articulatory deficits, assessed by the phonetic error proportion, were strongly correlated with the WAB fluency score as well as with the postcentral gyrus and supramarginal gyrus. The results regarding the grammatical deficits showed that the proportion of words in sentences is significantly related to the middle frontal gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus. Working memory is associated with the supramarginal gyrus, which is responsible for mapping phonology to articulation [22]. Phonetic articulatory planning and execution are related to the postcentral gyrus and supramarginal gyrus [28]. Several other research works have also shown that fluency is associated with the superior temporal gyrus [8], angular gyrus [1], precentral gyrus [15,18], anterior insula [4] and some white matter tracts [1,10,15,25], corresponding to the anterior segment of the arcuate fasciculus (AF), frontal aslant tract, uncinate fasciculus, superior longitudinal fasciculus and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus. In particular, the posterior superior temporal gyrus plays a role in phonological recognition, self-monitoring of speech and retrieval of semantic information [8]. The angular gyrus is involved in phonological production and retrieval [1]. The precentral gyrus supports executing speech mouth movements [15]. The anterior insula is a key area of the brain, which coordinates complex articulatory movements [4]. The above results were consistent with our study – except for those concerning the white matter tracts because we did not explore the relationship between white matter tracts and language function.
There are three subsets of auditory comprehension on the WAB scale, including single-word recognition, yes/no questions and sequential commands. Lwi et al. [26] used lesion-symptom mapping to investigate the relationship between specific lesion sites and these three subsets in patients with chronic aphasia. The results showed that the mid- to posterior middle and inferior temporal gyrus and the angular and inferior-middle occipital gyrus were related to poor performance on single-word recognition. The posterior superior temporal gyrus, mid- to posterior middle temporal gyrus and posterior inferior temporal gyrus were associated with poor performance on yes/no questions. The superior temporal gyrus, posterior middle temporal gyrus and angular gyrus were involved in poor performance on sequential commands. These results suggested that auditory comprehension deficits are mainly caused by injury of the superior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus and angular gyrus, especially injury of the posterior temporal lobe. However, the role of the posterior temporal lobe in comprehension remains controversial. Some studies have suggested that the anterior temporal lobe – rather than the posterior temporal lobe – is important for comprehension [13]. Since our study did not refer to the role of the anterior or posterior temporal lobe in comprehension, future studies are needed to further explore this issue. In addition to the areas mentioned above, some studies have also suggested that middle frontal gyrus [31], inferior frontal gyrus [20,27] and supramarginal gyrus [27,32] play a role in auditory comprehension. In particular, researchers have indicated that the middle frontal gyrus plays an important role in word comprehension [31]. The inferior frontal gyrus is associated with grammar, lexical retrieval and sentence comprehension [27]. The supramarginal gyrus is involved in auditory and phonological short-term memory related to sentence comprehension [32]. Regarding white matter tracts, Xing et al. [38] have shown that the uncinate fasciculus is related to word-level comprehension deficits. The posterior temporal white matter tracts, inferior longitudinal fasciculus and AF are related to sentence-level comprehension deficits. The inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus is related to both word- and sentence-level comprehension. These results were consistent with our results, except for those regarding the white matter tracts.
Repetition concerns auditory processing, phonological analysis, output mapping and speech production [5]. In one study, in order to investigate the neural correlates of repetition, Dell and colleagues [11] explored the connections between auditory input and phonological units as well as between the lexical system and phonological system. It turned out that the superior temporal gyrus, postcentral gyrus and supramarginal gyrus are associated with the process from the auditory input to phonological units and play a role in auditory discrimination. The precentral gyrus, postcentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus and insula are associated with lexical-phonological connections and are responsible for auditory-motor integration. Moreover, Baldo et al. [5] suggested that the middle temporal gyrus and angular gyrus are related to single-word repetition and are responsible for lexical semantic retrieval. Regarding white matter tracts, some studies have shown that the superior longitudinal fasciculus [35], inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus [9,40] and AF [9] are related to repetition. However, the role of the AF in repetition remains controversial. Some researchers believe that the AF is crucial for repetition [9], while others suggest that repetition is only associated with the cortex around perisylvian regions rather than the AF [5]. Therefore, the role of the AF in repetition needs to be further explored. These results were consistent with ours, except for those regarding the middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus and white matter tracts. One possible reason for this is that the previous research only involved word repetition and not sentence repetition, whereas we covered both with the WAB scale. Mirman et al. [28] have shown that grammatical sentence structuring relies on frontal regions, particularly the inferior and middle frontal gyrus. Schwart et al. [33] have also suggested that the middle frontal gyrus plays an important role in verbal working memory, which is essential for long sentence repetition.
To investigate the neural correlates of naming, one study divided naming into a semantic system and phonetic system [11]. The semantic system was the process from semantic units to lexical units, and the phonetic system was the process from lexical units to phonological units. The results showed that the semantic process was related to the anterior superior and middle temporal gyrus, temporal pole, middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus and angular gyrus. The phonetic system was related to the precentral gyrus, postcentral gyrus, supramarginal gyrus and insula. These results were the same as our results – except for those concerning the temporal pole because we did not include the temporal pole as an area of interest. Specifically, the anterior superior and middle temporal gyrus and angular gyrus are responsible for semantic representation, and the middle and inferior frontal gyrus play a role in the attentional or working memory processes that control such representations. In terms of white matter tracts, the superior longitudinal fasciculus [23], inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus [23], AF [23] and uncinate fasciculus [39] are related to naming ability.
There were some limitations in our study. First, the study was a retrospective analysis, so it was inevitable that there would be incomplete data, such as the data regarding stroke severity. Second, the sample size was small, with only 52 patients included. Third, we did not analyse the role of the right cerebral hemisphere in language because it was involved in only 5 of 385 patients in our study, nor did we study the effect of white matter tracts on aphasia. Fourth, we analysed the relationship between lesion site and language function but did not analyse the role of lesion volume because some studies have shown that the specific damaged structure rather than the lesion volume has been identified as a more accurate specific injury index [16,36]. In view of the above, prospective and large sample studies are needed in the future to further explore the relationship between lesion site and language function in patients with acute aphasia. Special attention should also be paid to the role of language function in the right cerebral hemisphere, the subcortical structure and the lesion volume.

Conclusions

Our study suggests that the brain is a complex network structure and that damage to different areas of the brain can lead to specific language impairments, which adds to our understanding of language pathology and provides a direction for future research and treatment.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the ethics committee of Hebei General Hospital.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Disclosure

The authors report no conflict of interest.

References

1. Alyahya R, Halai AD, Conroy P, Lambon RM. A unified model of post-stroke language deficits including discourse production and their neural correlates. Brain 2020; 143: 1541-1554.
2. Alyahya R, Halai AD, Conroy P, Lambon RM. Mapping psycholinguistic features to the neuropsychological and lesion profiles in aphasia. Cortex 2020; 124: 260-273.
3. An R, Liu GG. Cognitive impairment and mortality among the oldest-old Chinese. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016; 31: 1345-1353.
4. Baldo JV, Wilkins DP, Ogar J, Willock S, Dronkers NF. Role of the precentral gyrus of the insula in complex articulation. Cortex 2011; 47: 800-807.
5. Baldo JV, Katseff S, Dronkers NF. Brain regions underlying repetition and Auditory-Verbal short-term memory deficits in aphasia: Evidence from voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. Aphasiology 2012; 26: 338-354.
6. Bates E, Wilson SM, Saygin AP, Dick F, Sereno MI, Knight RT, Dronkers NF. Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Nat Neurosci 2003; 6: 448-450.
7. Berthier ML. Poststroke aphasia: epidemiology, pathophysiology and treatment. Drugs Aging 2005; 22: 163-182.
8. Borovsky A, Saygin AP, Bates E, Dronkers N. Lesion correlates of conversational speech production deficits. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45: 2525-2533.
9. Catani M, Mesulam M. The arcuate fasciculus and the disconnection theme in language and aphasia: History and current state. Cortex 2008; 44: 953-961.
10. Chouiter L, Holmberg J, Manuel AL, Colombo F, Clarke S, Annoni JM, Spierer L. Partly segregated cortico-subcortical pathways support phonologic and semantic verbal fluency: A lesion study. Neuroscience 2016; 329: 275-283.
11. Dell GS, Schwartz MF, Nozari N, Faseyitan O, Branch CH. Voxel-based lesion-parameter mapping: Identifying the neural correlates of a computational model of word production. Cognition 2013; 128: 380-396.
12. DePippo KL, Holas MA, Reding MJ. Validation of the 3-oz water swallow test for aspiration following stroke. Arch Neurol 1992; 49: 1259-1261.
13. Dronkers NF, Wilkins DP, Van Valin RJ, Redfern BB, Jaeger JJ. Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension. Cognition 2004; 92: 145-177.
14. Dronkers NF. A new brain region for coordinating speech articulation. Nature 1996; 384: 159-161.
15. Fridriksson J, Guo D, Fillmore P, Holland A, Rorden C. Damage to the anterior arcuate fasciculus predicts non-fluent speech production in aphasia. Brain 2013; 136: 3451-3460.
16. Gerstenecker A, Lazar RM. Language recovery following stroke. Clin Neuropsychol 2019; 33: 928-947.
17. Halai AD, Woollams AM, Lambon RM. Using principal component analysis to capture individual differences within a unified neuropsychological model of chronic post-stroke aphasia: Revealing the unique neural correlates of speech fluency, phonology and semantics. Cortex 2017; 86: 275-289.
18. Halai AD, Woollams AM, Lambon RM. Predicting the pattern and severity of chronic post-stroke language deficits from functionally-partitioned structural lesions. Neuroimage Clin 2018; 19: 1-13.
19. Hijikata N, Kawakami M, Wada A, Ikezawa M, Kaji K, Chiba Y, Ito M, Fujino E, Otsuka T, Liu M. Assessment of dysarthria with Frenchay dysarthria assessment (FDA-2) in patients with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Disabil Rehabil 2020; 1-8.
20. Kristinsson S, Thors H, Yourganov G, Magnusdottir S, Hjaltason H, Stark BC, Basilakos A, den Ouden DB, Bonilha L, Rorden C, Hickok G, Hillis A, Fridriksson J. Brain damage associated with impaired sentence processing in acute aphasia. J Cogn Neurosci 2020; 32: 256-271.
21. Kwah LK, Diong J. National institutes of health stroke scale (NIHSS). J Physiother 2014; 60: 61.
22. Lacey EH, Skipper-Kallal LM, Xing S, Fama ME, Turkeltaub PE. Mapping common aphasia assessments to underlying cognitive processes and their neural substrates. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2017; 31: 442-450.
23. Lee JK, Ko MH, Park SH, Kim GW. Prediction of aphasia severity in patients with stroke using diffusion tensor imaging. Brain Sci 2021; 11: 304.
24. Lee KB, Kim JS, Hong BY, Sul B, Song S, Sung WJ, Hwang BY, Lim SH. Brain lesions affecting gait recovery in stroke patients. Brain Behav 2017; 7: e868.
25. Li M, Zhang Y, Song L, Huang R, Ding J, Fang Y, Xu Y, Han Z. Structural connectivity subserving verbal fluency revealed by lesion-behavior mapping in stroke patients. Neuropsychologia 2017; 101: 85-96.
26. Lwi SJ, Herron TJ, Curran BC, Ivanova MV, Schendel K, Dronkers NF, Baldo JV. Auditory comprehension deficits in post-stroke aphasia: Neurologic and demographic correlates of outcome and recovery. Front Neurol 2021; 12: 680248.
27. Mesulam MM, Thompson CK, Weintraub S, Rogalski EJ. The Wernicke conundrum and the anatomy of language comprehension in primary progressive aphasia. Brain 2015; 138: 2423-2437.
28. Mirman D, Kraft AE, Harvey DY, Brecher AR, Schwartz MF. Mapping articulatory and grammatical subcomponents of fluency deficits in post-stroke aphasia. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2019; 19: 1286-1298.
29. Moon HI, Lee HJ, Yoon SY. Lesion location associated with balance recovery and gait velocity change after rehabilitation in stroke patients. Neuroradiology 2017; 59: 609-618.
30. Moon HI, Pyun SB, Tae WS, Kwon HK. Neural substrates of lower extremity motor, balance, and gait function after supratentorial stroke using voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. Neuroradiology 2016; 58: 723-731.
31. Otsuki M, Soma Y, Onodera O, Tsuji S, Satoh A, Yamada N. Auditory comprehension in transcortical motor aphasia due to a medial lesions of the left frontal lobe. No To Shinkei 1995; 47: 1081-1085.
32. Robson H, Griffiths TD, Grube M, Woollams AM. Auditory, phonological, and semantic factors in the recovery from wernicke’s aphasia poststroke: Predictive value and implications for rehabilitation. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2019; 33: 800-812.
33. Schwartz MF, Kimberg DY, Walker GM, Faseyitan O, Brecher A, Dell GS, Coslett B. Anterior temporal involvement in semantic word retrieval: Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping evidence from aphasia. Brain 2009; 132: 3411-3427.
34. Shewan CM, Kertesz A. Reliability and validity characteristics of the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB). J Speech Hear Disord 1980; 45: 308-324.
35. Sul B, Lee KB, Hong BY, Kim JS, Kim J, Hwang WS, Lim SH. Association of lesion location with Long-Term recovery in post-stroke aphasia and language deficits. Front Neurol 2019; 10: 776.
36. Sul B, Kim JS, Hong BY, Lee KB, Hwang WS, Kim YK, Lim SH. The prognosis and recovery of aphasia related to stroke lesion. Ann Rehabil Med 2016; 40: 786-793.
37. von Elm E,Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. Epidemiology 2007; 18: 800-804.
38. Xing S, Lacey EH, Skipper-Kallal LM, Zeng J, Turkeltaub PE. White matter correlates of auditory comprehension outcomes in chronic Post-Stroke aphasia. Front Neurol 2017; 8: 54.
39. Zhang J, Zheng W, Shang D, Chen Y, Zhong S, Ye J, Li L, Yu Y, Zhang L, Cheng R, He F, Wu D, Ye X, Luo B. Fixel-based evidence of microstructural damage in crossing pathways improves language mapping in Post-stroke aphasia. Neuroimage Clin 2021; 31: 102774.
40. Zhang J, Wei X, Xie S, Zhou Z, Shang D, Ji R, Yu Y, He F, Du Y, Ye X, Luo B. Multifunctional roles of the ventral stream in language models: Advanced segmental quantification in post-stroke aphasic patients. Front Neurol 2018; 9: 89.
Copyright: © 2022 Mossakowski Medical Research Centre Polish Academy of Sciences and the Polish Association of Neuropathologists. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0) License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), allowing third parties to copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format and to remix, transform, and build upon the material, provided the original work is properly cited and states its license.
Quick links
© 2024 Termedia Sp. z o.o.
Developed by Bentus.