eISSN: 2084-9885
ISSN: 1896-6764
Neuropsychiatria i Neuropsychologia/Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology
Bieżący numer Archiwum Artykuły zaakceptowane O czasopiśmie Rada naukowa Bazy indeksacyjne Prenumerata Kontakt Zasady publikacji prac
Panel Redakcyjny
Zgłaszanie i recenzowanie prac online
SCImago Journal & Country Rank
2/2014
vol. 9
 
Poleć ten artykuł:
Udostępnij:

Original article
The effect of basic neuropsychological interventions on performance of students with dyscalculia

Salar Faramarzi
,
Sima Sadri

Neuropsychiatria i Neuropsychologia 2014; 9, 2: 48–54
Data publikacji online: 2014/10/14
Plik artykułu:
- the effect.pdf  [0.10 MB]
Pobierz cytowanie
 
 

Introduction


The term “learning disabilities” was first put forward by Kirk (1963). On the other hand, it is stated that learning disabilities are a set of heterogeneous disorders which manifest in serious difficulty with acquiring and applying listening, speaking, reading, writing and calculating, and these disorders have neurological origins which have a developmental process that begins at preschool and continues until adulthood (Gartland and Stronsnider 2007).
It has also been mentioned that learning disability is a type of neuropsychological disability which creates a serious problem for reading, calculating and writing, and if it is not assessed well, it has a potential influence on maladaptive performance of individuals and creates difficulties in life’s dimensions and a neuropsychological assessment will be essential to determine the source of the problem (Emami 2010). On the other hand, some experts in the field of learning disabilities propose two views: 1) neuropsychological/developmental learning disabilities and 2) educational learning disabilities/achievement (Kirk et al. 2006).
Those who have chosen the neuropsychological/developmental view seek an internal cause for the students’ problems in subjects. Based on this division, biological/genetic, perceptual-motor, visual processing, auditory processing, memory and attention disorders are classified into neuropsychological/developmental learning disabilities (Shaleve and Gross Tsure 2007).
The term neuropsychological and neurological dyscalculia is used when the defect and deficiency in doing mathematics skills is a sign of developmental or abnormal delay in understanding one or more mathematical operations (Mazzocco 2001). In addition, the American Psychiatric Association (APA) uses dyscalculia for children whose mathematics skills, given the child’s age conditions and measured cognitive ability, are below the expectations. Thus, we assume that this problem, deviation or deferral in the process and expansion of mathematical concepts’ growth and skills’ achievements has happened in childhood. Regardless of this deviation or deferral, there is agreement that a neuropsychological origin exists in creating mathematical difficulties (Shaleve and Gross Tsure 2007).
The prevalence estimates of dyscalculia in students have been reported to be 5-8% (Rousselle and Noël 2007; Gersten et al. 2005) and nearly 5% in Tehran city (Ramezani 2001). It is supposed that there is a neuropsychological underpinning of dyscalculia which might be the result of genetic or non-genetic factors (Shaleve and Gross Tsure 2001).
One promising result of the conducted studies in the field of dyscalculia is the consistency of these findings in the association between neuropsychological characteristics and mathematical problems (Hale and Fiorello 2004). Three subtypes of dyscalculia have been suggested (Geary 1993). The first subtype is manifested by a deficit in verbal semantic memory (the memory related to the meaning of words) and leads to difficulties such as memorization and retrieval of arithmetic facts even after repetition and ample practice. This kind of disability seems to be associated with left hemisphere malfunction and is typically seen alongside a reading disorder.
The second subtype of dyscalculia includes using developmentally immature methods for solving mathematical problems and frequent errors in solving simple problems (Geary 2010; Geary et al. 1999). The third subtype of dyscalculia, which has lower prevalence, apparently includes a visuo-spatial skills defect and leads to inappropriate columnar alignment or place value errors (placing decimal points inappropriately).
In etiology, and designing and preparing instructional interventions, attention to the characteristics and neuropsychological profiles of these children is pivotal. Potential cognitive factors of neuropsychology are involved in causing the three above problems. Hence, there have been a considerable number of investigations into neuropsychological features of children with mathematics learning disabilities during the last few years (Hale and Fiorello 2004).
One of these children’s problems which has attracted the researchers’ and experts’ attention is the weakness of executive functions and attention that has been proved by a large number of studies (Semrud-Clikeman 2005; Fletcher et al. 2007; Meltz 2007; McCloskey et al. 2009; Geary 2010). Perception disorders (agnosia, discrimination and sense interpretation) have attracted the attention of experts in the field of learning disabilities. Some prevalent disorders in the field of perception are: visual reception disorder, visual discrimination, visual memory, auditory discrimination, auditory memory and sensory integration (Fletcher et al. 2007).
Other pieces of research have also demonstrated that students with dyscalculia have severe difficulties with language skills including phonological awareness, rapid automatic naming and speech production (Geary 1993; Bley and Thornton 2001; Geary 2004; Swanson and Jerman 2006). Moreover, research studies have shown that students with dyscalculia, in comparison to the normal group, have problems in visuo-spatial processing (Semrud-Clikeman 2005; Geary 2005).
Typically, students with dyscalculia have difficulty with memorizing auditory and visual stimulants. Swanson et al. (2009) concluded that memory scales can discriminate the students with learning disabilities from slow learners and average students. These researchers point out that the students with learning disabilities manifest dominant failures of active memory (short-term).
Numerous studies have demonstrated that these students have performance problems in memory functions such as active memory, memory for names, memory for faces, active visuo-spatial memory and long-term memory (Swanson and Jerman 2006; Jordan et al. 2007; Meyer et al. 2010).
Further, they are significantly weaker in acquiring and remembering mathematical concepts such as the facts and principles of mathematics, the concept of numbers, numeration, calculating and solving problems in comparison to normal students (Swanson et al. 2009; Jordan et al. 2010).
On the other hand, the studies conducted on brain hemispheres have suggested that mathematics is a reciprocal task and demands both right and left hemispheres’ activities (Menon et al. 2002), which might relate to the nature of mathematics’ assignments and reveal hemispherical differences, that is, patients with left hemisphere injuries encounter difficulty with calculation and patients with right hemisphere injuries face solely mathematics reasoning difficulties (Isaacs et al. 2001; Menon et al. 2002; Hale and Fiorello 2004; Geary 2010).
Additionally, the studies of Hale and Naglieri (2004) demonstrated that visual processing skills and processing speed anticipate mathematics achievement but the success variance in mathematics is associated with semantic and active memory. This view supports the belief that dyscalculia has a multiple neuropsychological origin. Also, the new imaging studies of neuropsychological processes in mathematics have illustrated that prefrontal and inferior parietal areas (which comprise angular and supramarginal gyri) are more involved in mathematics skills (Hale et al. 2003; Hale and Fiorello 2004; Varma and Schwartz 2007; Pennington 2009).
The research studies of Oreizi et al. (2005), Abedi et al. (2008), Mir Mehdi et al. (2009) and Abedi (2010) have shown that children with learning disabilities, particularly students with dyscalculia, have difficulties with neuropsychological aspects (executive functions, attention, language, visuo-spatial processing, memory and learning) and failure in neuropsychological skills can predict children’s learning disabilities. Semrud-Clickeman (2005) also found in their promising investigations that a substantial number of students with learning disabilities have abnormal brain waves.
Some researchers (Gersten et al. 2005; Dowker 2005; Swanson and Jerman 2006; McCloskey et al. 2009; Penington 2009; Meyer et al. 2010; Jordan et al. 2010; Geary 2010; Abedi 2010; Mazzocco and Hanich 2010) have pointed out the influence of neuropsychological interventions (e.g., instructing and reinforcing executive functions, attention, language skills, visuo-spatial processing and active memory) on improving academic progress of children with dyscalculia. Moreover, these researchers emphasize that with respect to mathematics assignments which have multiple neuropsychological underpinnings, neuropsychological interventions must include all aspects.
It can be inferred from the studies mentioned above that the foundation of academic problems of students with dyscalculia is disorders and difficulties in neuropsychological processes, and provided that neuropsychological interventions occur the performance of mathematics students will be improved. Therefore, the chief question of this investigation is: do neuropsychological interventions have an effect on improving the performance of students with dyscalculia? The hypothesis of the current research is that basic neuropsychological interventions influence the mathematics performance of students with dyscalculia.

Material and methods

Research methodology

The current research employed a quasi-experimental design with pretest, posttest and a control group. The independent variable and dependent variable are respectively neuropsychological interventions and academic performance of girl students (8-9 years old) with dyscalculia in mathematics.

Statistical population and sample

The research population consists of all dyscalculia girls students (8-9 years old) in the second grade of elementary school during the 2010-2011 academic years in Isfahan city of Iran. Using multi-stage sampling, we chose two elementary schools randomly from all 6 educational districts in Isfahan. Subsequently, one class from each elementary school was randomly selected according to the entry requirements for the research: 1) serious problems in learning mathematics (arithmetic) by considering Keymath test scores, academic achievement test of mathematics and educational result paper of first and second grade girl students (8-9 age) of elementary schools; 2) having an average or upper IQ level determined using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children (WISC); 3) enjoying physical and psychological health affirmed by a physician, counselor and teacher; 4) having an appropriate social, economic and cultural status. Thirty students with dyscalculia were randomly selected from the above-mentioned schools and divided into two groups, experimental (15) and control (15). Furthermore, parents were satisfied with their children’s participation and were informed about all the intervention stages. Eventually, neuropsychological interventions (independent variable) were conducted on the experimental group.

Research instruments

1. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III (WISC-III-R). This scale was designed by Wechsler in 1949; this scale was revised and named Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III (WISC-III-R) after standardization. Shahim (1998) standardized it in Iran.
2. Keymath test. This test was made by Conolly (1988); in terms of topic and order it is made up of three sections – basic concepts, operations and applications – and each section is classified into three or four areas. Mohammad Esmaeil and Hooman (1999) standardized this test by making use of Cronbach’s  for Iranian students of 6.6-8.11 years of age and the reliability was reported to be 0.57, 0.62, 0.67, 0.56, and 0.55. This test is considerably efficient at recognizing students with dyscalculia (Conolly 1988).

Procedures

After the pretest, in order to reinforce and instruct neuropsychological aspects (executive functions, attention, visuo-spatial processing, language, working memory), interventions were conducted in 10 sessions of 2 hours (2 sessions a week). Training the experimental group lasted for two months.
Neuropsychological interventions’ underpinning is activities which cause the stimulation and reinforcement of neuropsychological connections. These activities include:
1. Reinforcing active memory: exercises related to reinforcing auditory and visual memory, enhancing recognition memory by hidden objects, practice with meaningless words, numbers and recalling them, following orders in story style, and showing a movie.
2. Reinforcing attention: reinforcing visual attention by pictures in which a particular feature or object is among other distractors, reinforcing auditory attention by audiotape, maintenance and auditory attention (hearing a favorable excerpt without attending to the noise).
3. Training executive functions such as planning and organizing: instructing planning for short-term goals, performing and following plans to achieve the goals, classifying cubes and bars based on length, color and thickness, constructing the structure according to the model, storing and recalling the details associated with a math problem.
4. Developing and reinforcing visuo-spatial perception: doing exercises associated with reinforcing eye-hand coordination, doing balancing exercises, finding ways through a labyrinth, identifying geometric shapes and volumes without using the eyes, moving according to perceptive and balancing mosaics, recognizing shapes from the background, copying models by using graph paper.
5. Reinforcing the skills related to speech and language: auditory discrimination reinforcement by using Belz, auditory sensitivity reinforcement by using dark cans, phonological awareness, fitting words into texts, listening comprehension, understanding words and mathematics concepts.
At the end of each training session, some assignments were given as exercise to the parents and a few sports in parallel with intervention programs such as bowling for reinforcing the child’s attention, playing hula hoop and tire for reinforcing spatial perception, playing with dolls and attention to detail for reinforcing visual precision, preparing an audiotape for reinforcing auditory precision and storytelling were introduced.

Statistical data analysis

The research data were analyzed and interpreted by statistical criteria of average, variance, standard deviation and covariance analysis by making use of SPSS software.

Results

The principal research hypothesis was: neuropsychological interventions influence the academic performance of girl students (8-9 years old) with dyscalculia. To demonstrate the groups’ differences, first the average and standard deviation of experimental and control groups and then covariance analysis are presented.
As Table 1 shows, the average of the whole experimental group in the pretest stage was 5.97, and this average for the control group was 6. In the posttest, the average of all scores of mathematics tests for the experimental group and control group was respectively 10 and 5.67. Eventually in the follow-up stage, the total average of the experimental group is 9.13 and 5.6 for control group. These results are shown in Table 1.
As can be seen in Table 2, considering the pretest scores as the auxiliary variable, neuropsychological interventions have led to a significant difference between the experimental and control group (p < 0.001); the effect size is 0.79. That is, 79% of the posttest variance (math academic performance) is caused by neuropsychological interventions. Statistical power of 100 also proves the sufficiency of sample size. Therefore, it can be concluded that neuropsychological interventions have had an effect on improving mathematics academic performance of girl students (aged 8-9) with dyscalculia.

Discussion and conclusion

As stated, the aim of the current research was to examine neuropsychological interventions’ effectiveness on mathematics academic performance of students with mathematics learning disability. Covariance analysis showed that considering pretest scores as the auxiliary variable, neuropsychological interventions (reinforcing attention, executive functions in planning and organizing level, working memory, language skills and visuo-spatial processing) had an effect on augmentation and mathematics performance improvement of elementary students with mathematics learning disability. Consequently, the research findings prepare the ground to infer that mathematics is a bilateral task and needs the activity of both hemispheres (right and left) and strengthens the belief that students’ disabilities in learning mathematics have multiple neuropsychological foundations and hence it needs multilateral examination of neuropsychological aspects and multidimensional neuropsychological interventions. The research results are in line with Gersten et al. (2005), Dowker (2005), Geary (2005), Swanson and Jerman (2006), McCloskey et al. (2009), Pennington (2009), Geary (2010) and Mazzocco and Hanich (2010). These researchers have shown the efficacy of neuropsychological interventions in mathematics academic performance of children with mathematics learning disabilities. They have reported that the performance of elementary school children with mathematics learning disabilities in neuropsychological tests (executive functions, attention, memory and visuo-spatial processing) is drastically weaker than normal children. To explain the research findings, it can be said that children have to be proficient in a set of skills which are neuropsychological aspects such as attention, executive functions, language, visuo-spatial processing and memory. Normal children will learn these skills automatically whereas children with learning disabilities encounter difficulty with applying these skills and are not able to master them automatically. On the other hand, identifying the weak points of a child with learning disability in neuropsychological dimensions can lead to an effective educational plan and understanding the problem.
Highlighting the effect of neuropsychological interventions on improving mathematics performance of students, the results of these studies support the view that during the process of mathematical calculations different areas of the brain and hemispheres are involved and children do not use one method for solving arithmetic problems. In this view it is concluded that mathematical errors of children also follow different patterns. Accordingly, multilateral neuropsychological examinations are needed to evaluate a child.
In examining the history pertaining to the topic it has been realized that behavioral neurology is the root of neuropsychology and behavioral neurology is a branch of neurology which concerns performance disorders of high cognitive levels (such as language, understanding, and visual perception) (Zillmer et al. 2008). On the other hand, the main purpose of neuropsychological evaluation is that the information obtained from children’s behaviors is a reflection of the integration of the nervous system’s performance (Stinnett et al. 2002).
Studies on neuropsychological characteristics of learning disability provide a firm logic for using neuropsychological cures for these learner populations, since this approach results in renewal and improvement of sensory-motor and perceptive systems of a child.
Neuropsychology involves the idea that the person’s neuropsychological hardware determines the whole behaviors of a person and there are signs which indicate that stimulation or neuropsychological interventions cause changes in the brain’s performance. There are also signs that stimulation or intervention can cause changes in the brain’s performance (Zillmer et al. 2008).
The aim of early interventions’ effect with augmenting the stimulations is to release the potential abilities of an individual. Likewise, numerous re‑education efforts for people with stroke and traumatic brain injury (TBI) are the foundation of these hypotheses that the brain can improve to some degree.
Besides, the assumption of the neuropsychological approach is the mental ability to create a cause and effect relationship between the area of brain error and deviation in the student’s mathematics ability. To classify special areas, the psychiatrist attempts to show which parts are responsible for mathematics performance of learners who have difficulties with mathematics. Afterwards, with the cooperation of learning disabilities and neuropsychology experts they design a set of neuropsychological interventions.
In conclusion, although this research has limitations including limitation of the kinds of instruments and sample selection, it is suggested that this investigation be practiced on various learning disabilities according to gender, age and also demographic features. It is also recommended that the managers and teachers of pre-elementary and elementary schools design rich educational environments along with instructive play so that the nervous prerequisites of the children for growth in executive functions, attention, visuo-spatial processing, language and memory are more reinforced and ameliorated. To this end, attention to neuropsychological interventions as fundamental mathematics learning skills can be an effective approach in curing and improving the performance of children with mathematics learning disabilities.

References

1. Abedi A, Malekpour M, Molavi H, et al. A comparison of executive functions and attention in pre-elementary students with neuropsychological or developmental disabilities with normal children. Adv Cogn Sci 2008; 10: 38-49.
2. Abedi A. The effect of neuropsychological interventions on improving academic performance of children with mathematics learning disabilities. Adv Cogn Sci 2010; 12: 1-16.
3. Bley NS, Thornton CA. Anchoring adolescents understanding of math concepts in rich problem – solving environments. Remedial Spec Education 2001; 22: 299-314.
4. Conolly AJ. Keymath-revised: a diagnostic inventory of essential mathematics. American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN 1988.
5. Dowker A. Early identification and intervention for students with mathematics difficulties. J Learn Disabil 2005; 38: 324-332.
6. Emami M. Learning disorders of mathematics. J Except Education 2010; 49: 33-36.
7. Fletcher JM, Lyon GR, Fuchs S, Barnes MA. Learning disabilities: from identification to intervention. Guilford Press, New York 2007.
8. Gartland D, Stronsnider R. Learning disabilities and young children identification and intervention. Learn Disabil Quarterly 2007; 30: 63-72.
9. Geary DC. Mathematical disabilities: cognitive, neuropsychological and genetic components. Psychol Bull 1993; 114: 345-352.
10. Geary DC. Mathematics and learning disabilities. J Learn Disabil 2004; 37: 4-15.
11. Geary DC. Role of cognitive theory in the study of learning of learning disability in the mathematics. J Learn Disabil 2005; 38: 305-307.
12. Geary DC. Mathematical disabilities: reflections on cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. Learn Individ Differ 2010; 20: 130-133.
13. Geary DC, Hoard MK, Hamson CO. Numerical and arithmetical cognition: patterns of functions and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disability. J Exp Child Psychol 1999; 74: 213-239.
14. Gersten R, Jordan NC, Flojo JR. Early identification and interventions for students with mathematics difficulties. J Learn Disabil 2005; 38: 293-304.
15. Hale JB, Fiorello CA. School neuropsychology: a practitioner’s handbook. Guilford Press, New York 2004.
16. Hale JB, Fiorello CA, Bertin M, Sherman R. Predicting math achievement through neuropsychological interpretation of WISC-III variance components. J Psycho Educational Assess 2003; 21: 358-380.
17. Hale JB, Naglieri JA. Specific learning disability classification in the new individuals with disabilities education act: The danger of good ideas. School Psychologist 2004; 58: 6-14.
18. Hanich LB, Jordan NC, Kaplan D, Dick J. Performances across different areas of mathematical cognition in children with learning difficulties. J Educational Psychology 2001; 93: 615-626.
19. Isaacs EB, Edmonds CJ, Lucas A, Gadian DG. Calculation difficulties in children with very low birth weight. Brain 2001; 124: 1701-1707.
20. Jordan NC, Glutting J, Ramineni C. The importance of number sense to mathemathics achievement in first and third grades. Learn Individ Differ 2010; 20: 82-88.
21. Jordan NC, Hanich LB, Kaplan D. A longitudinal study of mathematical competencies in children with specific mathematics difficulties versus children with comorbid mathematics and reading difficulties. Child Dev 2003; 74: 834-850.
22. Jordan NC, Kaplan D, Hanich LB. Achievement growth in children with learning difficulties in mathematics: findings of a two-year longitudinal study. J Education Psychology 2007; 94: 569-597.
23. Kirk SA, Gallagher JJ, Anastasiw NJ, Coleman R. Education exceptional children. Houghton Mifflin, Boston 2006.
24. Mazzocco MM. Math learning disability and math LD subtypes: evidence from studies of Turner syndrome, fragile X syndrome, and neurofibromatosis type 1. J Learn Disabil 2001; 34: 520-533.
25. Mazzocco MM, Hanich LB. Math achievement, numerical processing, and executive function in girls with Turner syndrome (TS): do girls with Turner syndrome have math learning disability? Learn Individ Differ 2010; 20: 70-81.
26. McCloskey G, Perkins L, Divner B. Assessment and intervention for executive function difficulties. Routledge Press, New York 2009.
27. Meltz L (ed.). Executive function in education: from theory to practice. Guilford Press, New York 2007.
28. Menon V, Mackenzie K, Rivera SM, Reiss AL. Prefrontal cortex involvement in processing incorrect arithmetic equations: evidence from event related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 2002; 16: 119-130.
29. Meyer ML, Salimpoor VN, Wu SS, et al. Differential contribution of specific working memory components to mathematical achievement in 2nd and 3rd graders. Learn Individ Differ 2010; 20: 101-109.
30. Mir Mehdi SR, Alizadeh H, Seif Naraghi M. The influence of instructing executive functions on mathematics performance and reading of pre-elementary students with special learning disabilities. Res Quarterly Realm Except Child 2009; 9: 1-12.
31. Mohammad Esmaeil E, Hooman HA. Correspondence and standardization of Iran Keymath mathematics’ test. Res Quarterly Realm of Except Child 1999; 332: 4-323.
32. Oreizi H, Abedi A, Taji M. The relation between counting ability, visual attention, auditory perception and metacognitive knowledge with mathematics qualification in pre-elementary students of Isfahan city. Quarterly J Educ Innovations 2005; 4: 133-149.
33. Pennington BF. Diagnosing learning disorders: a neuropsychological framework. Guilford Press, New York 2009.
34. Ramezani M. The study of prevalence rate of dyscalculia in fourth and fifth grade students of Tehran schools. Research center of exceptional children, Tehran 2001.
35. Rousselle L, Noël MP. Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: a comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing. Cognition 2007; 102: 361-365.
36. Semrud-Clikeman M. Neuropsychological aspects for evaluating disabilities. J Learn Disabil 2005; 38: 563-568.
37. Shahim S. Wechsler Intelligence Revised Scale for Children, the agenda and norms (2nd ed.). Shiraz University Publications, 1998.
38. Shaleve RS, Gross Tsure V. Neuropsychological aspects of developmental dyscalculia. Mathematical Cognition 2001; 3: 105-120.
39. Shaleve RS, Gross Tsure V. Developmental dyscalculia is familiar learning disabilities. J Learn Disabil 2007; 34: 50-65.
40. Stinnett TA, Oehler Stinnett J, Fuqua DR, Palmer LS. Examination of the underlying structure of the NEPSY: a developmental neuropsychological assessment. J Psyhoeduc Assess 2002; 20: 66-82.
41. Swanson HL, Jerman O, Zheng X. Math disabilities and reading disabilities: can they be separated? J Psychoeduc Assess 2009; 27: 175-196.
42. Swanson HL, Jerman O. Math disabilities: a selective meta-analysis of the literature. Rev Educ Res 2006; 76: 249-274.
43. Varma S, Schwartz DL. Beyond dyscalculia: the neural bases of elementary school mathematics. J Learn Disabil 2007; 39: 371-379.
44. Zillmer EA, Spiers MV, Culbertson WC. Principles of neuropsychology. Thomson Woodworth, Belmont, CA 2008.
Copyright: © 2014 Termedia Sp. z o. o. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0) License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), allowing third parties to copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format and to remix, transform, and build upon the material, provided the original work is properly cited and states its license.
© 2024 Termedia Sp. z o.o.
Developed by Bentus.